David Spiegelhalter, Sex by numbers, London, Profile books, 2015
For many, sex is more about quality than
quantity, so David Spiegelhalter’s Sex by
numbers may put off many potential readers.
Yet, in sex quantity seems to have
a certain quality of its own –or so it claims Brooke Magnanti, invoking the
intellectual authority of Stalin. This book has indeed a quality of its own:
accuracy. Our folk understanding of
sex is full of made up numbers to which we inadvertently stick without further
reflection. Checking them out is more complicated that it seems: there are
competing sources and we need a certain degree of statistical (and
methodological) literacy to assess them properly. Hence, we can only be
grateful to have Spiegelhalter, a world-leading statistician, spelling out for
us what we really know about the numbers of sex in a clear and accessible
manner.
Spiegelhalter is a Bayesian: for him,
probabilities measure how strong our beliefs about random events are. This is a
technicality that readers may safely ignore, but it explains why the book
starts with a credibility ranking of the available figures about sex: numbers
we can believe, numbers that are reasonably accurate, numbers that could be out
by quite a long way, numbers that are unreliable, and numbers that have just
been made up. Evidence in the two first categories will improve our statistical
understanding of sex, whereas the remaining three scores will probably mislead
us.
Ranking evidence depends crucially on its
sources and half of this book is about how social research on sexuality can be
properly carried out. Spiegelhalter's paradigm for reliable data is the British
National Survey of Sexual Attitudes and Lifestyles (Natsal-3, 2010-2012). This
is based on a random sample of face-to-face interviews funded by a private
charity, the Wellcome trust -incidentally, the same trust commissioning this
book. Spigelhalter spends time discussing the methodology and comparative
reliability of his many sources (devoting an entire appendix to Natsal methods)
and the crucial choices on which they all depend (e.g., what counts as a sexual
partner). He also uses Natsal data in the first few chapters to introduce a
number of handy statistical concepts: the mean and the median are important
when we wonder about how much sex we are having.
With all these methodological caveats in sight,
Spiegelhalter proceeds to inform you about everything you thought you knew
about sex, despite not having a reliable source to check. Statistics about
partners, heterosexual and homosexual activity, masturbation, reproduction etc.
Most of it illustrated with graphics, about which I will make my only formal
complaint about the book: in the epub version, sometimes they were not easy to
read (despite trying the graphics on various readers). The text instead is
delightful to read. Spigelhalter excels at both clarity and wit, both in the best
British tradition, even if (or perhaps because) the topic is sex.
Spigelhalter is cautious, but not shy, in
appraising causality through data. Sometimes the evidence makes more likely
some explanations of why sex happens the way it does. But often the data are
far from conclusive regarding causation and, at best, they just describe what
we do (and how often we do it). Spiegelhalter
adopts a good old positivist stance regarding the science of sex and admits at
all points what we do not know, keeping it separate from any normative
judgment. The most opinionated readers may be displeased by such a sober
discourse on sex. The rest of us will be surely enlightened by the quality in
the quantity.
{August, 2016}
{Metapsychology}
{Metapsychology}
No hay comentarios:
Publicar un comentario